Welcome to SJGLE.com! |Register for free|log in
Welcome to SJGLE.com! |Register for free|log in
Related Searches: Tea Vitamin Nutrients Ingredients paper cup packing
Colour can impact the taste of food, and our experiences and expectations can affect how we taste food, according to Penn State researchers, who suggest this may have implications for how food and beverage industries should market their products.
"The colour of a beverage can impact how we think it should taste," said John E. Hayes, associate professor of food science and director, Sensory evaluation Center, Penn State. "For example, yellow is commonly associated with drinks that are more sour, like lemonade, wheras red is associated with drinks that are more sweet, like sports drinks."
The Penn State researchers wondered how people learn colour-taste associations, and whether people could be taught new colour-taste associations. They reported their findings online in Food Quality and Preference, and it will be published in the January print edition.
Also, the researchers wanted to determine why people enjoy the bitter taste in some foods but not others.
"Bitterness can indicate toxicity, but people still enjoy coffee, chocolate and IPA-style beers," said Molly J. Higgins, doctoral candidate in food sciences. "Why do people like some bitter compounds but not others?"
Hayes and Higgins wanted to find out if there were differences between different bitter compounds, and if people could taste those differences. If there are different types of bitter tastes, it could explain why some people like the bitterness of coffee, but not the bitterness of chocolate.
"There may be different kinds of bitterness, even if we dont have the language to describe it," said Hayes.
First, Hayes and Higgins needed to test whether they could teach people to associate specific tastes with specific colours. They created solutions that tasted bitter, sweet, sour or savory, and paired each one with a unique colour, such as green for sweet or red for bitter.
"We avoided preconceived colour-taste pairings, such as yellow for sour or red for sweet, because we didnt want to bias our sample," said Higgins.
The researchers had participants taste the coloured liquids over four different sessions, then gave the participants uncoloured solutions and asked them to taste and choose a colour for each.
Participants matched the correct colour and taste 59% of the time, significantly higher than the researchers expected from random chance, which would have been 25%. It surprised researchers how quickly some participants learned to associate new colours with different tastes.
"This was only after four exposures," said Higgins. "We were surprised to see some participants correctly match every sample in the post test."
After showing that they could teach new colour-taste associations to their participants, researchers then tested whether people could distinguish between three different bitter tastes –– caffeine, quinine, and Tetralone, a hop extract found in beers. Hayes and Higgins repeated the experiment with a new group of study subjects and assigned unique colours to each bitter chemical, such as yellow for caffeine and green for quinine. However, after four lab visits, participants could not match the colours to their corresponding bitter compounds any better than expected by chance.
Although the research did not get the results they expected, they still were intrigued by how quickly some participants learned new colour-taste associations in the first experiment. Hayes and Higgins continue to study how people associate taste with colour, noting that while some participants were quick learners, others were not.
"This finding could indicate that some people learn colour-taste associations well, while others might be more resistant to changes or new colour-taste associations," said Higgins. "This might have implications in the food industry if a company were to launch a new flavoured product with a colour. Some consumers might not learn or accept a new colour and flavour pairing as well as others."
The U.S. Department of Agriculture Hatch Project and Penn State supported this work. Kalsec supplied materials for this research.
E-newsletter
Tags
Latest News